--- Make It Stick - Highlights

Make It Stick

By Brown, Peter C.

37 highlights

Empirical research into how we learn and remember shows that much of what we take for gospel about how to learn turns out to be largely wasted effort.

Page 9Preface

If learners spread out their study of a topic, returning to it periodically over time, they remember it better. Similarly, if they interleave the study of different topics, they learn each better than if they had studied them one at a time in sequence.

Page 11Preface

Learning is deeper and more durable when it’s effortful. Learning that’s easy is like writing in sand, here today and gone tomorrow.

Page 181. Learning Is Misunderstood

We are poor judges of when we are learning well and when we’re not.

Page 181. Learning Is Misunderstood

Rereading text and massed practice of a skill or new knowledge are by far the preferred study strategies of learners of all stripes, but they’re also among the least productive.

Page 181. Learning Is Misunderstood

Retrieval practice—recalling facts or concepts or events from memory—is a more effective learning strategy than review by rereading.

Page 191. Learning Is Misunderstood

Retrieval strengthens the memory and interrupts forgetting. A single, simple quiz after reading a text or hearing a lecture produces better learning and remembering than rereading the text or reviewing lecture notes.

Page 191. Learning Is Misunderstood

While the brain is not a muscle that gets stronger with exercise, the neural pathways that make up a body of learning do get stronger, when the memory is retrieved and the learning is practiced.

Page 191. Learning Is Misunderstood

if you practice elaboration, there’s no known limit to how much you can learn. Elaboration is the process of giving new material meaning by expressing it in your own words and connecting it with what you already know

Page 221. Learning Is Misunderstood

mental model is a mental representation of some external reality

Page 241. Learning Is Misunderstood

the elements that shape your intellectual abilities lie to a surprising extent within your own control.

Page 261. Learning Is Misunderstood

What’s apparent from the research is that gains achieved during massed practice are transitory and melt away quickly.

when it comed to mere exposure such as rereading

Page 301. Learning Is Misunderstood

Learning is stronger when it matters, when the abstract is made concrete and personal

Page 331. Learning Is Misunderstood

It makes sense to reread a text once if there’s been a meaningful lapse of time since the first reading, but doing multiple readings in close succession is a time-consuming study strategy that yields negligible benefits at the expense of much more effective strategies that take less time

Page 401. Learning Is Misunderstood

the ease with which they follow the argument gives them the feeling that they already know it and don’t need to study it.

Page 421. Learning Is Misunderstood

Mastery requires both the possession of ready knowledge and the conceptual understanding of how to use it.

Page 461. Learning Is Misunderstood

The good news is that we now know of simple and practical strategies that anybody can use, at any point in life, to learn better and remember longer: various forms of retrieval practice, such as low-stakes quizzing and self-testing, spacing out practice, interleaving the practice of different but related topics or skills, trying to solve a problem before being taught the solution, distilling the underlying principles or rules that differentiate types of problems, and so on.

Page 521. Learning Is Misunderstood

Reflection can involve several cognitive activities that lead to stronger learning: retrieving knowledge and earlier training from memory, connecting these to new experiences, and visualizing and mentally rehearsing what you might do differently next time.

Page 602. To Learn, Retrieve

Since as far back as 1885, psychologists have been plotting “forgetting curves” that illustrate just how fast our cranberries slip off the string. In very short order we lose something like 70 percent of what we’ve just heard or read. After that, forgetting begins to slow, and the last 30 percent or so falls away more slowly, but the lesson is clear: a central challenge to improving the way we learn is finding a way to interrupt the process of forgetting.

memory involved delaying the forgetting curve

Page 622. To Learn, Retrieve

practicing retrieval makes learning stick far better than reexposure to the original material does. This is the testing effect, also known as the retrieval-practice effect

Page 632. To Learn, Retrieve

In 1978, researchers found that massed studying (cramming) leads to higher scores on an immediate test but results in faster forgetting compared to practicing retrieval.

Page 692. To Learn, Retrieve

multiple sessions of retrieval practice are generally better than one, especially if the test sessions are spaced out.

Page 702. To Learn, Retrieve

teacher as I might think I am, my teaching is only a component of their learning, and how I structure it has a lot to do with it, maybe even more.

tesching is only a com¶onent of learning. the way its taught and tested matters

Page 822. To Learn, Retrieve

Studies show that giving feedback strengthens retention more than testing alone does, and, interestingly, some evidence shows that delaying the feedback briefly produces better long-term learning than immediate feedback.

- [ ] link this to Peak note about deliberate practice

Page 842. To Learn, Retrieve

In the classroom, delayed feedback also yields better long-term learning than immediate feedback does.

Page 852. To Learn, Retrieve

While any kind of retrieval practice generally benefits learning, the implication seems to be that where more cognitive effort is required for retrieval, greater retention results.

Page 862. To Learn, Retrieve

Students whose study strategies emphasize rereading but not self-testing show overconfidence in their mastery.

Page 882. To Learn, Retrieve

Practice at retrieving new knowledge or skill from memory is a potent tool for learning and durable retention. This is true for anything the brain is asked to remember and call up again in the future—facts, complex concepts, problem-solving techniques, motor skills.

Page 912. To Learn, Retrieve

Effortful retrieval makes for stronger learning and retention.

Page 912. To Learn, Retrieve

Repeated retrieval not only makes memories more durable but produces knowledge that can be retrieved more readily, in more varied settings, and applied to a wider variety of problems.

Page 912. To Learn, Retrieve

Testing doesn’t need to be initiated by the instructor. Students can practice retrieval anywhere; no quizzes in the classroom are necessary. Think flashcards—

Page 922. To Learn, Retrieve

Students who take practice tests have a better grasp of their progress than those who simply reread the material.

Page 922. To Learn, Retrieve

Giving students corrective feedback after tests keeps them from incorrectly retaining material they have misunderstood

Page 932. To Learn, Retrieve

The mixing of problem types, which boosted final test performance by a remarkable 215 percent, actually impeded performance during initial learning.

Page 1033. Mix Up Your Practice

What would interleaved practice look like? You practice procedure 1 just a few times, then switch to procedure 4, then switch to 3, then to 7, and so on.

Page 1033. Mix Up Your Practice

The learning from interleaved practice feels slower than learning from massed practice. Teachers and students sense the difference.

Page 1033. Mix Up Your Practice

The basic idea is that varied practice—like tossing your beanbags into baskets at mixed distances—improves your ability to transfer learning from one situation and apply it successfully to another.

Page 1053. Mix Up Your Practice